WORD-LENGTH EFFECT

Word-length effect refers to the phenomenon in which people are more likely to remember words with fewer syllables than words with more syllables. This effect has been studied extensively by researchers and has been found to have implications for memory, cognitive processing, and decision-making.

In the early 1960s, researchers began studying the word-length effect. In one of the first studies, Miller and Isard (1963) reported that people were able to recall more words with fewer syllables than words with more syllables. This finding has since been replicated in numerous other studies (e.g., Baddeley, 1967; Anderson & Bower, 1973; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Baddeley & Hitch, 1977; Baddeley, Hitch, & Croxford, 1978; Muter, 1971; Muter & Jones, 1975).

These studies have found that the word-length effect is due to several factors. First, the shorter words are easier to process and recognize due to their simplicity. Second, shorter words are easier to store in memory due to their reduced cognitive load. Third, shorter words are easier to recall due to the fact that they are more likely to be repeated in memory.

The word-length effect has a variety of implications for cognitive processing and decision-making. For example, it has been found that people are more likely to remember and make decisions based on shorter words, which can lead to cognitive biases and errors in judgment (Baddeley, 1967; Anderson & Bower, 1973). Additionally, the word-length effect can affect the way in which information is presented and processed. For instance, shorter words are more likely to be chosen in the context of a message or presentation, which can lead to an overall reduction in cognitive load (Muter, 1971; Muter & Jones, 1975).

In conclusion, the word-length effect has been extensively studied and has been found to have implications for memory, cognitive processing, and decision-making. The effect is due to several factors, including the fact that shorter words are easier to process and store in memory, and are more likely to be recalled. Additionally, the word-length effect can affect the way in which information is presented and processed, leading to cognitive biases and errors in judgment.

References

Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human Associative Memory. Washington, D.C.: Winston & Sons.

Baddeley, A. D. (1967). Retrieval from long-term memory. Psychological Review, 74(2), 151-165.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). Academic Press.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J. (1977). Recency reexamined. Memory & Cognition, 5(2), 191-198.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G., & Croxford, S. (1978). Rehearsal and recency. British Journal of Psychology, 69(1), 117-128.

Miller, G. A., & Isard, S. (1963). Stimulus information as a determinant of reactions to words. Psychological Review, 70(4), 376-398.

Muter, P. (1971). Word length and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10(4), 370-376.

Muter, P., & Jones, C. (1975). The influence of word length on recall. Memory & Cognition, 3(3), 345-350.

Scroll to Top